Ayr Brane

Psychology Bits & Bytes

Pages

  • AYR BRANE
  • PSYCHOLOGY
  • BOOKS
  • VIDEOS
  • SUPPORT LINKS
Showing posts with label Memory. Show all posts
Showing posts with label Memory. Show all posts

Friday, 29 April 2011

Scientists find way to wipe out painful memories

by Danny Buckland, Daily Mirror 29/04/2011

It sounds like something from a science fiction movie. But researchers reckon they have found a way to erase painful memories and post-traumatic stress. They discovered a link between a protein called PKM and recollections of disturbing incidents.

By targeting the specific brain circuit which holds the tormenting memory they believe they could weaken it or wipe it out. The incredible study paves the way for treatment for war veterans and victims of horrific attacks. It may also help drug addicts and people with long-term memory disorders such as Alzheimer’s.

Professor David Glanzman, the study’s senior author, said: “I think it will be feasible."

Read more: http://www.mirror.co.uk/news/health-news/2011/04/29/scientists-fiind-way-to-wipe-out-painful-memories-115875-23092965/#ixzz1Ktvduclf
Posted by Journoway No comments:
Email ThisBlogThis!Share to XShare to FacebookShare to Pinterest
Labels: Memory, Trauma

Thursday, 21 April 2011

Time to spring clean... your mind? Scientists say memory lapses can be blamed on too much irrelevant information


By Fiona Macrae, Mail Online
Last updated at 8:00 AM on 21st April 2011

If you struggle to remember names and numbers or frequently fail to follow the plot of a film, help could be at hand. Scientists say the problem is that you know too much – and you need to declutter, or spring-clean your mind.

Experiments show that the memory lapses that come with age are not simply due to brain slowing down. Instead, they can be blamed on the well-used brain finding it more and more difficult to stop irrelevant information interfering with the task in hand.
The first step in the study was to compare the working memory of the young and old. Working memory involves holding information in mind while manipulating it mentally.

Examples in everyday life include retain plots of films and books to understand or predict what will happen next and following the thread of a conversation while working out how you can contribute to the topic.
In the context of the study, it involved giving the volunteers groups of sentences and asking them to work out whether each line made sense – and to remember the last word of each sentence.
Rest: Getting a good night's sleep is just one way to spring clean the mind

Stay sharp by playing music

Overall, the younger people, who had an average age of 23, did better, the Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology reports. The Canadian researchers then did a second experiment to see what was hindering  the older volunteers, who had an average age of 67. This involved being shown a pictures of eight animals and being asked to memorise the order in which the creatures appeared. The volunteers were then shown dozens of the pictures and asked to click on their computer mouse when the first animal in their memorised sequence occurred, then the second and so on.

The older adults found it more difficult to progress, suggesting the previous picture was stuck in their mind. Mervin Blair, of Montreal’s Concordia University, said: ‘We found that  the older adults had more difficulty in getting rid of previous information. ‘We found that that accounted for a lot of the working memory problems seen in  the study.’

A third study confirmed that the memory problems were not simply due to a  simple slowing down of the mind. Mr Blair, a PhD candidate, says that the older mind appears to have trouble  suppressing irrelevant information. This makes it more difficult to concentrate on the here and now.

Read more: http://www.dailymail.co.uk/sciencetech/article-1378991/Memory-loss-solution-Declutter-mind.html#ixzz1K8vinag5
Posted by Journoway No comments:
Email ThisBlogThis!Share to XShare to FacebookShare to Pinterest
Labels: Ageing, Brains, Memory

Friday, 21 January 2011

Expert board game players utilise specific brain areas

BBC NEWS Health

21 January 2011 Last updated at 13:06

Scientists have discovered that expert board game players use a part of their brain that amateurs fail to utilise. The research, published in Science, involved scanning the brains of both professional and amateur Japanese "Shogi" players. Shogi is a Japanese game, similar to chess.

Scientists from the RIKEN Brain Science Institute in Japan said that intuitive playing was probably not due to nature, but brain training. Shogi is a very popular game in Japan, played to professional level. Professional players train for up to ten years, three to four hours a day to achieve the level of expertise needed to play professionally.

Intuitive decisions

They are able to make very quick "intuitive" decisions about which move in any combination on the board, would produce the best outcome.

The researchers recruited 30 professional shogi players from the Japanese Shogi Association. They also had a control group of amateur players. The professional players were presented with a game of shogi already in progress and given 2 seconds to choose the next best move - from a choice of four moves. The researchers found that there were significant activations in the caudate nucleus area of the brains of professional players while they were making their quick moves.

Brain activity

In contrast, when amateur players were asked to quickly find the next best move, there was no significant activation in the caudate nucleus. This brain activity was specific to professional players who were making quick decisions about the next best move.

In addition, professionals did not use that area of the brain when they were given a longer time of 8 seconds, to think strategically about further moves they could make. In this scenario, the caudate nucleus area of the brain was not activated.

The caudate nucleus area of the brain was historically thought to be involved with the control of voluntary bodily movements. However more recently it has also been associated with learning and memory.

Related stories

  • Mind chemical 'controls choice'
  • Brain's adventure centre located

http://www.bbc.co.uk/news/health-12250687
Posted by Journoway No comments:
Email ThisBlogThis!Share to XShare to FacebookShare to Pinterest
Labels: Brain Training, Brains, Chess, Learning, Memory, Shogi

Wednesday, 19 January 2011

False memories of sexual abuse lead to terrible miscarriages of justice


Chris French
 
guardian.co.uk, Thursday 25 November 2010 12.17 GMT

To avoid the innocent being convicted, police, lawyers and judges must understand the fickle nature of human memory 
 

Patients with common psychological problems such as depression and anxiety are sometimes subjected to 'memory recovery' techniques. 

 

Many of those working in our legal system have such a poor understanding of the nature of human memory that miscarriages of justice are an almost inevitable consequence, according to a book published today by the British False Memory Society. Miscarriage of Memory, edited by William Burgoyne, Norman Brand, Madeline Greenhalgh and Donna Kelly, presents factual accounts of prosecutions in the UK that were based entirely upon memories of sexual abuse recovered during therapy in the absence of any supporting evidence.

Typically such cases occur when a vulnerable individual seeks help from a psychotherapist for a commonly occurring psychological problem such as anxiety, depression, low self-esteem, and so on. At this stage, the client has no conscious memories of ever being the victim of childhood sexual abuse and is likely to firmly reject any suggestion of such abuse. To a particular sort of well-meaning psychotherapist, however, such denial is itself evidence that the abuse really did occur.

Despite strong criticism from experimental psychologists, many psychotherapists still accept the Freudian notion of repression – the idea that when someone experiences extreme trauma, a defence mechanism kicks in that buries the memory of the traumatic event so deep that it cannot be retrieved into consciousness. Like radioactive waste, its presence is said to exert a malign influence. Indeed, the whole rationale of such therapy is that these hidden memories must be recovered and worked through in order to achieve psychological health.
 

During therapy, and often as a result of "memory recovery" techniques such as hypnotic regression and guided imagery, the client may gradually develop clear and vivid memories of abuse having taken place, typically at the hands of parents and other family members.

On the evidence of a huge amount of well-controlled research, we can now be confident that these memory recovery techniques are highly likely to give rise to false memories – apparent memories for events that never took place.

The memories can be detailed and extremely bizarre, involving ritualised Satanic abuse, gross acts of sexual perversion, cannibalism, human and animal sacrifice, and so on. But they may be nothing more than fleeting images. Indeed, some patients never manage to recover explicit "memories" of abuse but are convinced that such abuse must have occurred because their therapist, who is perceived as an authority figure, tells them that it is the only explanation for their unhappiness.

Whether the patient "recovers" explicit memories or not, the end result will be a family torn apart, with all the heartache, confusion and lasting emotional damage that entails.

It is, of course, far more likely that such cases will come to the attention of the legal system when explicit memories are involved. The book Miscarriage of Memory details several such cases.

Understandably, practitioners and social workers are legally required to inform the authorities when they suspect that sexual abuse has occurred and, equally understandably, the need to protect possible victims of abuse is paramount. Serious problems can arise, however, when the initial suspicions of abuse are not well-founded and when the legal system is in the hands of people who often do not fully appreciate the complexities of human memory.
 

Consider, for example, the comments of retired judge Gerald Butler, when asked whether we needed memory experts to explain to juries how people's memories work (speaking on "Can you trust your memory?" the BBC Radio 4 programme Law in Action in 2008):
"I think, frankly, that is a faintly ridiculous suggestion. We do have experts who can be very helpful ... there are handwriting experts, there are fingerprint experts, and of course there are the DNA experts who have turned out to be of immense value in the courts. But we also have juries who are there in order to use their common sense and when it is a situation that you weigh up a witness's evidence and decide whether he or she is telling the truth or that he or she has a faithful recollection of what has taken place, this is essentially a matter for the jury. It is not a matter for an expert."
In fact, many hundreds of people have been wrongfully convicted in the UK because juries and those involved in the legal system relied upon "common sense" in considering issues relating to memory. Several thousand case histories have been referred to the British False Memory Society and at least 672 of these are known to have involved the police or higher legal authorities.

It is imperative that those working in the legal system are familiar, at least in general terms, with the way that memory works. Experimental psychologists, following the initial controversy over the veracity of recovered memories back in the 1980s, have developed several reliable techniques to study factors that influence the formation and maintenance of false memories. The studies have proved beyond doubt that false memories can be produced quite readily in susceptible individuals.

Of course, false memories do not only arise in the context of sexual abuse allegations. As Professor Tim Valentine, an expert in psychology and the law at Goldsmiths, University of London, informs me:
"Witnesses' recall can be influenced by information acquired during an investigation. Just repeatedly questioning a witness tends to increase their confidence in both correct and mistaken answers. A shopkeeper who was a key witness in the Lockerbie bomb case was interviewed 20 times by the police, during which he was shown fragments of burnt clothing. He recalled a Libyan customer who had been in the shop nine months previously. Initially he said he did not sell the man any shirts. In court he described selling two shirts to the customer that were similar to fragments of clothing found in the suitcase that contained the bomb. Might this be a false memory induced by questioning about the shirts?"
Another dramatic case further illustrates the way in which witnesses can sometimes confuse the source of their memories, with potentially catastrophic results. Donald Thomson, an Australian psychologist, was bewildered when the police informed him that he was a suspect in a rape case, his description matching almost exactly that provided by the victim. Fortunately for Thomson, he had a watertight alibi. At the time of the rape, he was taking part in a live TV interview – ironically, on the fallibility of eyewitness testimony. It turned out that the victim had been watching Thomson on TV just before the rape occurred and had confused her memory of him with that of the rapist.
 

Well-controlled experiments have also shown conclusively that memory can become contaminated when co-witnesses discuss their recall of events, a phenomenon known as "memory conformity". Valentine provides one possible example of this in a high-profile British murder case in 1999:
"When Barry George first stood on an identity parade on suspicion of murdering Jill Dando, one witness identified him, but two neighbours made no identification. These three witnesses were given a lift home together. During the journey they discussed the identity parade and learned that the witness had identified number 2 in the line-up. Subsequently the other two witnesses made a statement identifying number 2. These 'partial identifications' were given as evidence in court. Barry George's conviction was quashed at his second appeal."
A huge amount of well-controlled research and analysis of myriad real-life legal cases have shown that to understand the complexity of human memory requires rather more than just "common sense".

Chris French is a professor of psychology at Goldsmiths, University of London, where he heads the Anomalistic Psychology Research Unit. He edits the The Skeptic

Further reading 


Brainerd, CJ, & Reyna, VF (2005) The Science of False Memory. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Loftus, EF, & Ketcham, K (1994)
The Myth of Repressed Memory: False Memories and Allegations of Sexual Abuse. New York: St Martin's Press.
McNally, RJ (2003) Remembering Trauma. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press.
Ofshe, R., & Watters, E (1994) Making Monsters: False Memories, Psychotherapy, and Sexual Hysteria. New York: Scribner.
Schacter, DL (2001) The seven sins of memory: How the mind forgets and remembers. Boston: Houghton Mifflin.


Source: Guardian
http://www.guardian.co.uk/science/2010/nov/24/false-memories-abuse-convict-innocent

Posted by Journoway No comments:
Email ThisBlogThis!Share to XShare to FacebookShare to Pinterest
Labels: False Memories, Memory, Psychotherapy, Repression
Older Posts Home
Subscribe to: Posts (Atom)

Search This Blog

Blog Archive

  • ▼  2013 (6)
    • ▼  February (6)
      • Surfing the internet for long periods of time can ...
      • New study: are we all living in the future now?
      • Can spicy food really give you nightmares?
      • Skeptics Brains Are Different
      • Science Confirms The Obvious: Not Everyone Can Be ...
      • No title
  • ►  2011 (56)
    • ►  May (2)
    • ►  April (5)
    • ►  March (3)
    • ►  February (1)
    • ►  January (45)

InfoList #1

  • http://shift.org.uk
  • http://www.centreformentalhealth.org.uk/across_mh/stigma.aspx
  • http://www.jrf.org.uk
  • http://www.mentalhealthsupport.co.uk
  • http://www.mind.org.uk
  • http://www.rethink.org
  • http://www.samaritans.org
  • http://www.sane.org.uk/what_we_do/support/helpline/
  • http://www.soterianetwork.org.uk
  • http://www.supportline.org.uk
  • http://www.time-to-change.org.uk

InfoList #2

  • http://www.anti-bullyingalliance.org.uk
  • http://www.bullyonline.org.uk
  • http://www.childline.org.uk
  • http://www.napac.org.uk/
  • http://www.papyrus-uk.org/
  • http://www.youtube.com/user/survivorway

InfoList #3

  • http://www.refuge.org.uk/
  • http://womensaid.org.uk/
  • http://whorls.angelfire.com/factorsinviolence.html
  • http://www.bullyonline.org
  • http://www.lucieblackmantrust.org
  • http://www.suzylamplugh.org/
  • http://www.crimestoppers-uk.org/crime-prevention/helping-prevent-crime/personal-safety
  • http://www.nss.org.uk
  • http://tansal.50megs.com/violencecoercive.html

Labels

  • Addiction (1)
  • Afterlife (1)
  • Ageing (1)
  • Aggression (1)
  • Agression (1)
  • Alcohol (1)
  • Amygdala (1)
  • Anxiety (1)
  • Autism (1)
  • Behaviour (1)
  • Beliefs (1)
  • Belonging (1)
  • Bill Maloney (1)
  • Brain Training (1)
  • Brains (7)
  • Bully Online (1)
  • Bullying (1)
  • CEOP (1)
  • Chess (1)
  • Child Abuse (1)
  • Child Protection (2)
  • Children (1)
  • CHRIS (1)
  • Chris Wittwer (1)
  • Community (1)
  • Consciousness (1)
  • Context (1)
  • Culture (1)
  • Cyber Bullying (1)
  • Depression (1)
  • Disabilities (1)
  • Divorce (1)
  • DNA (2)
  • Domestic Violence (3)
  • Dopamine (1)
  • Emotion (2)
  • Empathy (1)
  • Environment (1)
  • ESP (1)
  • False Memories (1)
  • Family Courts (1)
  • Full Moon (1)
  • Ganzfeld (1)
  • Genetic (1)
  • Hate Crime (2)
  • Hearing Voices (1)
  • Heritability (1)
  • Homo Sapiens (1)
  • Hypnosis (1)
  • Impulsivity (1)
  • Institutional Child Abuse (1)
  • Internet (3)
  • Kid Shield (1)
  • Kidscape (1)
  • Kipling Williams (1)
  • Labels (1)
  • Learning (1)
  • Lie to Me (1)
  • Lies (2)
  • Mad Pride (1)
  • Maturity (1)
  • Media (1)
  • Mediation (1)
  • Memory (4)
  • Mental Health (1)
  • Microexpressions (1)
  • Missing from Home (1)
  • Music (1)
  • One Upmanship (1)
  • Ostracism (1)
  • Paranormal (1)
  • Paul Ekman (1)
  • Phenomena (1)
  • Pie N Mash Films (1)
  • Problems (1)
  • Psychic Research (1)
  • Psychotherapy (1)
  • PTSD (1)
  • Quantum (1)
  • Red Mist (1)
  • Religion (2)
  • Repression (1)
  • Runaways (1)
  • Safety (1)
  • Samaritans (1)
  • Scapegoating (2)
  • Sherry Turkle (1)
  • Shogi (1)
  • Social Influence (1)
  • Social Networks (3)
  • Sound (1)
  • Stalking (1)
  • Status (1)
  • Stigma (1)
  • Stress (1)
  • Striatum (1)
  • Support (1)
  • Telepathy (1)
  • Teleport (1)
  • Trauma (2)
  • Truth (1)
  • Unfair Advantage (2)
  • Video Games (1)
These Sites are about beliefs and freedoms, and how people may believe or act - or even be treated - in certain ways.

As always, readers need to be cautious and responsible about taking things on board.
Postings are made in the hope they are of interest, or provoke further thought or discussion - and above all SAFETY!

Caveat Lector - Reader beware.

All information on this Blog is offered in the hope it may help you sort out what is best in your situation. No-one can tell what the consequences of anything may be, so find out what you can before taking any action.

Behaviour Not-in-Character

Behaviour Not-in-Character

Unseen Aspects of Behaviour

Unseen Aspects of Behaviour

BookBending

BookBending
Unusual books or slants

Finding Help

Finding Help
Tansal Abuse & Rights

In The Ayr

In The Ayr
Beliefs & Phenomena

Louise Ayr on Twitter

Louise Ayr on Twitter
Travel theme. Powered by Blogger.